The
authors test EndlerÕs sensory drive hypothesis. They
predict that in order to enhance conspicuousness of an important visual signal
used in male-male competition, the coloration of male damselfly species will
contrast most with the habitat during their peak activity period. The activity of
6 sympatric species of Enallagma was measured and
irradiance spectra were formed to characterize ambient light at the hours of
peak activity. To depict the coloration of males, reflectance spectra were
taken from the meso- and metapleural surface of the thorax and abdominal segments
of each individual. Reflectance of primary background vegetation on the pond
surface and shore were recorded as well. Subsequently,
the authors utilized these measurements to formulate mean values for chromatic
and achromatic contrast under 2 different models of odonate vision: a
trichromatic model and a tetrachromatic model.
They
found that the 3 species most active during midday had peak reflectances between 430nm and 470nm (Ôthe blue speciesÕ).
Similarly the 3 species most active near dusk, reached their max. reflectances
near 700nm (Ôlong wavelength speciesÕ). All but one Ôlong-wavelength speciesÕ showed
higher color contrast during their peak activity time under the tetrachromatic
model of vision. All the Ôblue speciesÕ showed higher contrast during their
most active time regardless of the vision model used. Moreover, all but one
species of damselfly studied experienced greatest achromatic contrast during their
respective periods of highest activity. Thus, all but one
species of damselfly demonstrated a drastic deviation of body color from
background/ambient light conditions. Furthermore, the eyes of male Ôlong-wavelength
speciesÕ have red, orange and yellow screening pigments that serve to amplify
transmission of long-wavelengths while the males of Ôblue speciesÕ lack these
screening pigments This evidence further supports the idea that the visual
systems of Ôblue speciesÕ and Ôlong wavelength speciesÕ are specially tuned to
their specific light environments.
What
we learned in class about effective visual signaling is the basis of EndlerÕs
theory of sensory drive. We discussed that the design and function of a signal
relies heavily on that signalÕs visual background; this background includes
light form the sun, reflected light from nearby signals, and reflected light
from the sky. Accordingly, for a signal to stick out and thus reach the receiver
effectively, it should maximize contrast with this background. We learned that
perception of contrast depends not only on the contrast of hue (the particular
wavelength that is selectively reflected) but also on that of brightness which
describes the intensity of a specific hue (contrast can also be enhanced by
visual patterns or movement). This justifies the authors in measuring both
chromatic and achromatic contrast of male coloration. Since the intensity and spectral
composition of ambient light changes throughout the day, this means that maximal
visual contrast is influenced by temporal variation of the habitat as well.
Thus, the model of visual signal design helps explain the use of two different
color signals in sympatric species of damselflies whose peak activity periods
have divergent light environments.