Prey
species have evolved two different strategies to avoid predation by either
having cryptic coloration, or aposematic coloration.
The first technique utilizes camouflaging coloring, while the second uses
bright warning coloration of unpalatability to
predators. Generally, conspicuous coloration is considered to be an efficient
signal, in that it is easy for the recipient to detect, and memorize. In
addition, bright warning coloration is often paired with patterns of
contrasting color. The functional benefit of patterns are not fully understood,
however, many hypotheses have been suggested. One hypothesis suggests that
patterns help trigger the memory in avoidance learning. Another hypothesis
suggests that patterns increase signal detection by creating many contrasting
edges via lateral inhibition.
Aronsson and Gamberale-Stille
conducted a study in order to determine the significance of patterns in aposematic prey selection by domestic chicks, Gallus gallus domesticus. The authors initially trained the chicks to
forage for mealworm prey. Then the chicks were subjected to trials where bright
coloration and two different patterns were used to indicate unpalatable prey
which was soaked in quinine. Palatable prey items were placed atop grey
rectangles of paper, whereas unpalatable prey were
always placed atop cyan colored paper, that had either a striped or dotted
pattern. Chicks were divided into two groups. In the seven discrimination
learning trials, group 1 was subject to grey positive stimuli, and cyan
negative stimuli with stripes. Group 2 was subject to the same positive
stimuli, and cyan negative stimuli with dots. These two groups were then
subject to a generalization test in which each group was divided into three
subgroups (e.g. 1a, 1b, 1c, and 2a, 2b, 2c). The subgroups were subject either
to the same stimuli as in training, a Òcolor onlyÓ stimulus, or a Òpattern
onlyÓ stimulus.
The
chicks were placed in cages with 32 prey options in sequence, half of which
were palatable and half of which were not. The number of each prey type
attacked by each individual was recorded. Based on the results, it was
determined that the chicks showed no difference in discrimination learning
between the two different patterns, and that in fact they responded
significantly to Òcolor onlyÓ negative stimuli, but not to Òpattern onlyÓ
negative stimuli. The pattern appeared to have more of a silent effect in terms
of prey choice, whereas bright color was the primary learned signal.
In
class we discussed how signals evolve. Initially a signal must evolve, such as
patterning, and must then be associated with a condition, such as unpalatability. Receivers of the signal must then be able
to learn and associate the signal with the condition, and then modify their
decision making based on the info received from the signal. This study
demonstrated that while color is clearly a signal used by chicks to determine
which prey to consume, a contrasting pattern cannot necessarily be considered a
signal because it is not clear if or how it is used by the receiver. Although
this study failed to determine the role patterns play in aposematic
coloration, the possibility exists that contrasting patterns evolved for
different reasons, such as functioning as a mating signal. However, patterning
is fairly ubiquitous among aposematicly colored
creatures. Therefore, patterning may be used as a signal by older predators
more than younger ones. Further research will have to be conducted to determine
if predators of different age groups further distinguish aposematicly
colored prey.
Aronsson, M., & Gamberale-Stille, G. (2008). Domestic chicks primarily attend to colour, not pattern,
when learning an aposematic coloration. Animal Behaviour,
75, 417-423.