Hanley D. & Doucet S.M. (2009). ÒEgg coloration in reing-billed gulls (Larus delawarensis): a
test of the sexual signaling hypothesis.Ó Behav Ecol Sociobiol (63): 719-729.
The
authors attempt to test whether the Ôsexual signaling hypothesisÕ explains the
presence of bright blue/green eggs in ring-billed gulls. Despite the increased conspicuousness
to predators, the authors predict that the blue color has evolved as a signal
of female quality to male conspecifics. The authors investigated the
applicability of this hypothesis by testing four of its predictions and
assumptions: 1) blue/green pigment is limiting so egg chroma will decrease with
laying order 2) clutch chroma is positively related to measures of female quality
3) brightness of blue green chroma should correlate positively with quality of
the offspring and 4) males invest more in those clutches of higher blue green
chroma.
The
study examined a total of 81 eggs. Three reflectance measures were made on each
egg to approximate their overall chroma and an average chroma for each clutch
was calculated. Also, fresh egg
mass, length and breadth of each egg and weight of chicks was calculated to
measure offspring quality. Furthermore, adults were captured and several measurements
of body size were made. Subsequently, blood was drawn for analysis of heterophil to lymphocyte ratio which was used as a proxy for female immune
stress. Finally, paternal investment in eggs was measured in relation to clutch
chroma, and entire clutches were swapped for 15 pairs of nests in order to
investigate whether paternal investment was influenced by some female signal
that co-varies with chroma of clutch. Observations of males assessed paternal
investment by measuring feeding rates, brooding bouts, threats to
neighbors and long call rate.
The
results showed that there was no expected negative association between egg
laying order and egg chroma; in fact, they found that the second eggs had
higher blue green chroma than the other eggs. Similarly, no relationship was
found between female quality (i.e.
body size and the proxy for immune stress) and average clutch chroma. Furthermore,
it was shown that blue green chroma did not correlate with offspring quality.
Finally, similar negative results were found for the association between male
investment and clutch chroma.
Although
the results did not support their hypothesis, the authorsÕ methods and clear predictions
reflect well the information presented in our discussion of signal costs. We
learned that conspicuous signals such as bright egg coloration accrue the large
cost of increased predation risk. We also discussed the prominence of honest
signaling which explains the authorsÕ prediction that the possession of the
pigments used to color eggs should be costly to the female if it truly
reflected female quality. Furthermore, we discussed that sexual selection and
natural selection are often at odds with each other, so it makes sense that the
authors suspected that sexual selection must have influenced the evolution of
such a conspicuous signal. This possible conflict between natural and sexual
selection is prevalent as we spoke of several examples including the ormia and tungara frog situations in
which this was the case. Furthermore, as mentioned in lecture the accurate
assessment of all potential costs and
benefits of a signal is more complicated than it would seem thus the inability
of the study to find the expected predictions does not necessarily take support
away from the Ôsexual signaling hypothesisÕ; it is difficult to know which
selective forces are most important so the creation of proper predictions is
sometimes difficult. For example, if predation risks have recently increased,
this might explain the lack of male preference for blue eggs; predation risk may
influence mate choice as we discussed in class with the guppy example.