Signal Evolution
References
Aspbury, A., and R. M. Gibson. 2004. Long-range visibility of greater
sage grouse leks: a GIS-based analysis. Animal Behaviour 67:1127-1132.
Bergstrom, C. T., and M. Lachmann. 2001. Alarm calls as costly signals
of antipredator vigilance: the watchful babbler game. Animal Behaviour
61:535-543.
Dabelsteen, T. 2004. Strategies that facilitate or counter
eavesdropping on vocal interactions in songbirds. Anais Da Academia Brasileira
De Ciencias 76:274-278.
De Kort, S. R., and C. Ten Cate. 2004. Repeated decrease in vocal
repertoire size in Streptopelia doves. Animal Behaviour 67:549-557.
Elias, D. O., A. C. Mason, and R. R. Hoy. 2004. The effect of substrate
on the efficacy of seismic courtship signal transmission in the jumping spider
Habronattus dossenus (Araneae : Salticidae). Journal of Experimental Biology
207:4105-4110.
Endler, J. A., and A. L. Basolo. 1998. Sensory ecology, receiver biases
and sexual selection. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 13:415-420.
Fullard, J. H. 2001. Auditory sensitivity of Hawaiian moths
(Lepidoptera : Noctuidae) and selective predation by the Hawaiian hoary bat
(Chiroptera : Lasiurus cinereus semotus). Proceedings of the Royal Society of
London Series B-Biological Sciences 268:1375-1380.
Garcia, C. M., and E. Ramirez. 2005. Evidence that sensory traps can
evolve into honest signals. Nature 434:501-505.
Gill, S. A., and S. G. Sealy. 2004. Functional reference in an alarm signal
given during nest defence: seet calls of yellow warblers denote brood-parasitic
brown-headed cowbirds. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 56:71-80.
Hare, B., M. Brown, C. Williamson, and M. Tomasello. 2002. The
domestication of social cognition in dogs. Science 298:1634-1636.
Kamo, M., S. Ghirlanda, and M. Enquist. 2002. The evolution of signal
form: effects of learned versus inherited recognition. Proceedings of the Royal
Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 269:1765-1771.
Kedar, H., M. A. Rodriguez-Girones, S. Yedvab, D. W. Winkler, and A.
Lotem. 2000. Experimental evidence for offspring learning in parent-offspring
communication. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological
Sciences 267:1723-1727.
Kingston, T., and S. J. Rossiter. 2004. Harmonic-hopping in Wallacea's
bats. Nature 429:654-657.
Krakauer, A. H. 2005. Kin selection and cooperative courtship in wild
turkeys. Nature 434:69-72.
Lachmann, M., S. Szamado, and C. T. Bergstrom. 2001. Cost and conflict
in animal signals and human language. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences of the United States of America 98:13189-13194.
Laiolo, P., J. L. Tella, M. Carrete, D. Serrano, and G. Lopez. 2004.
Distress calls may honestly signal bird quality to predators. Proceedings of
the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 271:S513-S515.
Madden, J. R., and K. Tanner. 2003. Preferences for coloured bower
decorations can be explained in a nonsexual context. Animal Behaviour
65:1077-1083.
Martins, E. P., A. Labra, M. Halloy, and J. T. Thompson. 2004.
Large-scale patterns of signal evolution: an interspecific study of Liolaemus
lizard headbob displays. Animal Behaviour 68:453-463.
Noldeke, G., and L. Samuelson. 2003. Strategic choice handicaps when
females seek high male net viability. Journal of Theoretical Biology 221:53-59.
Ord, T. J., and D. T. Blumstein. 2002. Size constraints and the
evolution of display complexity: why do large lizards have simple displays?
Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 76:145-161.
Ord, T. J., D. T. Blumstein, and C. S. Evans. 2002. Ecology and signal
evolution in lizards. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 77:127-148.
Peake, T. M., A. M. R. Terry, P. K. McGregor, and T. Dabelsteen. 2002.
Do great tits assess rivals by combining direct experience with information
gathered by eavesdropping? Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series
B-Biological Sciences 269:1925-1929.
Pie, M. R. 2005. Signal evolution in prey recognition systems.
Behavioural Processes 68:47-50.
Podos, J. 2001. Correlated evolution of morphology and vocal signal
structure in Darwin's finches. Nature 409:185-188.
Pruden, A. J., and G. W. Uetz. 2004. Assessment of potential predation
costs of male decoration and courtship display in wolf spiders using video
digitization and playback. Journal of Insect Behavior 17:67-80.
Scheuber, H., A. Jacot, and M. W. G. Brinkhof. 2004. Female preference
for multiple condition-dependent components of a sexually selected signal.
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences
271:2453-2457.
Searby, A., P. Jouventin, and T. Aubin. 2004. Acoustic recognition in
macaroni penguins: an original signature system. Animal Behaviour 67:615-625.
Shelley, E. L., and D. T. Blumstein. 2005. The evolution of vocal alarm
communication in rodents. Behavioral Ecology 16:169-177.
Smith, C., L. Barber, R. J. Wootton, and L. Chittka. 2004. A receiver
bias in the origin of three-spined stickleback mate choice. Proceedings of the
Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 271:949-955.
Spencer, K. A., K. L. Buchanan, A. R. Goldsmith, and C. K. Catchpole.
2003. Song as an honest signal of developmental stress in the zebra finch
(Taeniopygia guttata). Hormones and Behavior 44:132-139.
Tu, H. W., and L. L. Severinghaus. 2004. Geographic variation of the
highly complex Hwamei (Garrulax canorus) songs. Zoological Studies 43:629-640.
Walther, B. A., and D. H. Clayton. 2005. Elaborate ornaments are costly
to maintain: evidence for high maintenance handicaps. Behavioral Ecology
16:89-95.
Zimmer, M., O. Diestelhorst, and K. Lunau. 2003. Courtship in
long-legged flies (Diptera : Dolichopodidae): function and evolution of
signals. Behavioral Ecology 14:526-530.