Cultural Evolution Theory
April 6, 2000
*Indicates a paper with less mathematical notation and more prose relative to other
papers
Everyone Read This Paper
*Laland, K. N., P. J. Richerson, and R. Boyd. 1996. Developing a Theory of Animal
Social Learning. Pages 129-154 in C. M. Heyes and B. G. Galef, eds. Social
learning in animals the roots of culture. Academic Press, San Diego.
Cultural Evolution
*Boyd, R. and P. J. Richerson. 1985. Ch 9:
Conclusion. Pages 280-299 in R. Boyd and P. J. Richerson, eds. Culture and
the evolutionary process. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.
*Boyd, R. and P. J. Richerson. 1988. An Evolutionary Model of Social Learning: The
Effects of Spatial and Temporal Variation. Pages 29-48 in T. R. Zentall and
B. G. Galef, eds. Social learning psychological and biological perspectives. Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ.
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. and M. W. Feldman. 1983. Paradox of the evolution of communication
and of social interactivity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 80:2017-21.
Abstract: Communication between individuals of a species is likely to increase the
capacity to acquire skills useful for survival and propagation and thus may confer
important selective advantages. Since interaction occurs between two or more individuals,
the selective process is frequency dependent, and the analysis shows that communication
cannot initially increase at a reasonable rate when it is limited to random unrelated
individuals, so that it is likely to abort for stochastic reasons. However, this
bottleneck is removed if the communication process takes place in the nuclear family
or among close relatives or if aggregation of communicators occurs because of assortative
mating or meeting. Use of the individual conditional fitnesses we have introduced
earlier permits an exact analysis. We show that, in general, the initial rate of
increase can be geometric if and only if, in the class of selective models considered,
the conditional probability of a communicator interacting with another contains a
positive constant term. In our discussion of communication, cost factors for the
act of communication have been omitted. However, the model has been generalized to
include cooperativeness, and also altruism, or competition, by introducing costs.
There is a close relationship among these situations, and the same considerations
about the initial bottleneck and its resolution also extend to them. The models given
here are for haploids but they extend to diploids and the conclusions are similar.
Findlay, C. S., C. J. Lumsden, and R. I. Hansell.
1989. Behavioral evolution and biocultural games: vertical cultural transmission.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 86:568-72.
Abstract: We consider an evolutionary game model in which strategies are transmitted
culturally from parents to offspring rather than inherited biologically. Our analysis
yields two noteworthy results. First, biocultural games show a greater diversity
of dynamical behaviors than their purely biological counterparts, including multiple
fully polymorphic equilibria. Second, biocultural games on average exhibit greater
equilibrium strategy diversity because of the countervailing influences of cultural
transmission and natural selection. Therefore, knowledge of a strategy's influence
on Darwinian fitness is not sufficient to infer the evolutionary consequences of
biocultural games. Further, our results suggest that cultural transmission in the
presence of natural selection may be an important mechanism maintaining behavioral
diversity in natural populations.
*Pocklington, R. and Best, M. L. Cultural
Evolution and Units of Selection in Replicating Text. Journal of Theoretical Biology.
1997 Sep 7; 188(1):79-87.
Abstract: The use of biological models and metaphors in studies of culture has a
long and checkered history. While there are many superficial similarities between
biological and cultural evolution, attempts to pin down such analogies have not been
wholly successful. One limiting factor may be a lack of empirical evidence that the
basic assumptions of the evolutionary model are met within a cultural system. We
argue that a focus on the detection and description of the units of selection is
an essential first step in constructing any evolutionary model. In this paper we
outline the necessary connection between units of selection and evolution, describe
the properties of a unit of selection, and introduce an empirical method for the
detection of putative units of selection in a model cultural system: discourse within
NetNews, a discussion system on the Internet. (C) 1997 Academic Press Limited.
*Gatherer, D. and McEwan, N. R. On Units of Selection in Cultural Evolution. J Theor
Biol. 1998 Jun 7; 192(3):409-413.
Takahasi, K. 1998. Evolution of transmission
bias in cultural inheritance. J Theor Biol 190:147-59.
Abstract: Evolution of transmission bias in cultural inheritance is investigated
using simple models of cultural selection. Conventional models of cultural transmission
describe cultural changes by incorporating transmission bias and non-vertical pathways
into the ordinary population genetic framework. The methodology has been successful
in understanding cultural changes in terms of natural selection, but it is difficult
to see from the theoretical framework how biased transmission in favor of maladaptive
traits might have evolved. To show that ordinary cultural processes lead at times
to the evolution of a preference that favors a deleterious cultural variant, this
study presents an alternative model of cultural transmission, where cultural elements
are transmitted in a manner more like infections in epidemiological transmission.
An ordinary equilibrium analysis indicates that, under certain conditions, runaway
dynamics emerges and the coevolution of a maladaptive cultural variant and an associated
preference in favor of the maladaptive variant is observed. If the preference of
an individual does not change during its ontogeny (e.g., if it is transmitted genetically),
however, then cultural selection alone does not produce such runaway dynamics, and
only those preferences that favor adaptive variants should eventually evolve. Since
cultural processes may at times result in a reduction in the fitness of individuals,
simplistic adaptive interpretations of culture are unconvincing without detailed
specification of the cultural processes involved. Moreover, cultural runaway of this
kind may help to explain the existence of traits that are apparently maladaptive
at the individual level but may be advantageous for the group. Inferences are also
made regarding the observed differences between human and non- human social information
transfer.
Takahasi, K. 1999. Theoretical aspects of the mode of transmission in cultural inheritance.
Theor Popul Biol 55:208-25.
Abstract: This study investigates how evolutionary factors interact to determine
the relative importance of vertical versus nonvertical mode of transmission in cultural
inheritance. Simple mathematical models are provided to study the joint evolution
of two cultural characters, one determining the viability and the fertility of individuals,
and the other determining the vertical transmission rate of the first trait. Ordinary
local stability analyses indicate that intrademic processes should lead to a greater
reliance on vertical cultural transmission. On the other hand, when newly arisen
variants are adaptive and favored in biased cultural transmission, interdemic processes
may lead to a decrease in vertical transmission. This is because biased nonvertical
transmission may effectively propagate the adaptive variants, further increasing
the average growth rate of the population. These results are verified under several
distinct sets of assumptions. It is also inferred that the degree and intensity of
transmission bias may be the important determinants of cultural processes. Copyright
1999 Academic Press.
Wallace, R. and Wallace, R. G. Organisms, Organizations
and Interactions: an Information Theory Approach to Biocultural Evolution. Biosystems.
1999 Aug; 51(2):101-119.
Abstract: The language metaphor of theoretical biology, proposed by Waddington in
1972, provides a basis for the formal examination of how different self-reproducing
structures interact in an extended evolutionary context. Such interactions have become
central objects of study in fields ranging from human evolution-genes and culture-to
economics-firms, markets and technology. Here we use the Shannon-McMillan Theorem,
one of the fundamental asymptotic relations of probability theory, to study the 'weakest'
and hence most universal, forms of interaction between generalized languages. We
propose that the co-evolving gene-culture structure that permits human ultra- sociality
emerged in a singular coagulation of genetic and cultural 'languages', in the general
sense of the word. Human populations have since hosted series of culture-only speciations
and coagulations, events that, in this formulation, do not become mired in the 'meme'
concept. (C) 1999 Elsevier Science Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.
Gene-Culture Coevolution
Aoki, K. and Feldman, M. W. Toward a theory for the evolution of cultural communication:
coevolution of signal transmission and reception. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1987
Oct; 84(20):7164-8.
Abstract: A haploid sexual two-locus model of gene-culture coevolution is examined,
in which a dichotomous phenotype subject to natural selection is transmitted vertically
with probabilities dependent on the chosen parent's genotype and phenotype and the
offspring's genotype. Stability conditions for the genetically monomorphic corner
equilibria are obtained. In a specialization of this general model, one locus controls
the transmission and the other controls the reception of adaptive information. The
corner and edge equilibria of this doubly coevolutionary model are fully analyzed,
and conditions for transmission and reception to coevolve are derived in terms of
the efficiency of vertical transmission, the selective advantage gained from possessing
the information, the costs of transmission and reception, and the recombination fraction
between the two loci. Possible applications of the model are to the evolution of
semantic alarm calls in vervet monkeys and the phonetic aspects of human language.
In a third model with diploid genetics, we consider the initial increase of cultural
transmission from a mutation-selection balance in which the adaptive phenotype is
the consequence of a dominant gene at one locus. A second gene controls the transmission
of the phenotype in such a way that a new mutant at this second locus permits learning
of the adaptive phenotype from a parent who has it. This new mutant cannot increase
when rare.
Aoki, K. and Feldman, M. W. A gene-culture coevolutionary model for brother-sister
mating. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1997 Nov 25; 94(24):13046-50.
Abstract: We present a gene-culture coevolutionary model for brother-sister mating
in the human. It is shown that cultural--as opposed to innate-- determination of
mate preference may evolve, provided the inbreeding depression is sufficiently high.
At this coevolutionary equilibrium, sib mating is avoided because of cultural pressures.
*Avital, E. and Jablonka, E. Social-Learning
and the Evolution of Behavior. Animal Behaviour. 1994 Nov; 48(5):1195-1199.
Abstract: In animals capable of learning from a parent or other individual, socially
acquired behaviour can be transmitted through several generations. When the inheritance
of variations in such behaviour is independent of genotypic variations, natural selection
can operate on an additional level. Direct evolution of behaviour becomes possible,
and this may alter the estimates of costs and benefits of behaviour patterns for
the individual who transmits them. It is suggested that the effects of maternally
transmitted behaviour contribute to the evolution of maternal behavioural strategies,
and to the evolution of behaviour associated with male-female conflict.
*Feldman, M. W. and Laland, K. N. Gene-culture coevolutionary theory. Trends in Ecology
& Evolution. 1996 Nov; 11(11):453-457.
Abstract: Gene-culture coevolutionary theory is a branch of theoretical population
genetics that models the transmission of genes and cultural traits from one generation
to the next, exploring how they interact. These models have been employed to examine
the adaptive advantages of learning and culture, to investigate the forces of cultural
change, to partition the variance in complex human behavioral and personality traits,
and to address specific cases in human evolution in which there is an interaction
between genes and culture.
Feldman, M. W. and Zhivotovsky, L. A. Gene-culture coevolution: toward a general
theory of vertical transmission. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1992 Dec 15; 89(24):11935-8.
Abstract: A general formulation of cultural and genetic transmission is developed.
The cultural transmission is vertical and the genetics may involve multiple loci.
Each individual is represented by a phenogenotype, and conditions are given under
which the evolutionary dynamics of phenogenotype frequencies are reducible to phenogametic
or phenoallelic frequencies. The interaction between genes and culture is specified
by an association measure, and results on the order of magnitude of this association
at equilibrium are presented.
Findlay, C. S. Phenotypic Evolution Under Gene-Culture Transmission in Structured
Populations. Journal of Theoretical Biology. 1992 Jun 7; 156(3):387-400.
Laland, K. N. Sexual Selection With a Culturally
Transmitted Mating Preference. Theoretical Population Biology. 1994 Feb; 45(1):1-15.
Abstract: Culturally transmitted mating preferences may generate sexual selection
in human and protocultural animal species if they influence the intensity of selection
on genetically transmitted physical and behavioral traits. Haploid and diploid two-
''locus'' models of sexual selection are presented in which mating preferences are
culturally transmitted, while traits are transmitted genetically. The models exhibit
dynamics similar to those of conventional haploid models of sexual selection, generating
neutrally stable curves of equilibrium trait and preference frequencies. A culturally
transmitted preference that reaches a significant frequency through cultural drift,
individual learning, or social transmission can drag a less viable trait to fixation,
or non-zero frequencies. Simulations suggest that strong biases in the transmission
of preferences could take initially rare, less viable traits to fixation in as few
as 20 to 50 generations, and weak biases in less than 100 generations. These conclusions
hold for both biparental and maternally inherited mating preferences. Given the pervasiveness
of cultural influences on human mate choice, the analysis suggests that this interaction
may have played an important role in human evolution. (C) 1994 Academic Press, Inc.
Laland, K. N., J. Kumm, J. D. Van Horn, and
M. W. Feldman. 1995. A gene-culture model of human handedness. Behav Genet 25:433-45.
Abstract: A model of handedness incorporating both genetic and cultural processes
is proposed, based on an evolutionary analysis, and maximum-likelihood estimates
of its parameters are generated. This model has the characteristics that (i) no genetic
variation underlies variation in handedness, and (ii) variation in handedness among
humans is the result of a combination of cultural and developmental factors, but
(iii) a genetic influence remains since handedness is a facultative trait. The model
fits the data from 17 studies of handedness in families and 14 studies of handedness
in monozygotic and dizygotic twins. This model has the additional advantages that
it can explain why monozygotic and dizygotic twins and siblings have similar concordance
rates, and no hypothetical selection regimes are required to explain the persistence
of left handedness.
Takahasi, K. and Aoki, K. 2-Locus Haploid and Diploid Models for the Coevolution
of Cultural Transmission and Paternal Care. American Naturalist. 1995 Nov; 146(5):651-684.
Abstract: The coevolution of cultural transmission and paternal care is investigated
using two-locus haploid and diploid population genetic models. Maternal care of offspring
is the rule in mammals, whereas paternal involvement is often minimal. If the biological
father also provides care and his continued presence facilitates the transfer of
adaptive cultural information, then the conditions for the initial spread of a genetic
capacity for cultural transmission are easily satisfied. Conversely, a genetic tendency
for the biological father to provide care rather than to desert his mate is more
likely to evolve if his role in enculturation is especially important. These predictions
derived from the mathematically simpler haploid model are seen to hold approximately
for the more realistic diploid model. If the reliability of paternity is low, so
that faithful males often direct care and cultural information at unrelated infants,
the evolution of cultural transmission and paternal care are both unlikely to occur.
We speculate that the evolution of cultural transmission and the nuclear family may
have been linked in the hominid line.
Others (not recommended for reading; either old, duplicates of ideas, or difficult
to read)
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. and Feldman, M. W. Cultural transmission and evolution: a quantitative
approach. Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press; 1981.
Kumm, J. and M. W. Feldman. 1997. Gene-culture coevolution and sex ratios: II. Sex-chromosomal
distorters and cultural preferences for offspring sex. Theor Popul Biol 52:1-15.
Abstract: Cultural preferences for the sex of offspring may produce behavior, such
as female infanticide, sex-selective abortion and sex-selective parental investment,
which alter the sex ratio in a population. Empirical evidence suggests that some
genetic sex-ratio distorters are located on the sex chromosomes. Interactions between
cultural preferences and sex-linked sex-ratio distorters are examined. Criteria for
the spread of cultural preferences and sex-chromosomal distorter alleles are derived
analytically, and the coevolution of preferences and distorters is examined through
numerical iteration. Evolutionary equilibria and trajectories of gene-culture interactions
involving sex- chromosomal distorter alleles may produce severely male- or female-
biased primary sex ratios and adult sex ratios in populations. Adult sex ratios,
primary sex ratios, allele frequencies and the prevalence of cultural preferences
in the population are sensitive to initial conditions and cultural transmission parameters.
During the coevolutionary process phenoallelic association is observed in many cases
and is associated with unusual dynamics.
Kumm, J., K. N. Laland, and M. W. Feldman. 1994. Gene-culture coevolution and sex
ratios: the effects of infanticide, sex-selective abortion, sex selection, and sex-biased
parental investment on the evolution of sex ratios. Theor Popul Biol 46:249-78.
Abstract: The evolutionary consequences of culturally transmitted practices that
cause differential mortality between the sexes, thereby distorting the sex ratio
(e.g., female infanticide and sex-selective abortion), are explored using dynamic
models of gene-culture coevolution. We investigate how a preference for the sex of
offspring may affect the selection of genes distorting the primary sex ratio. Sex-dependent
differences in mortality have been predicted to select for a male- or female-biased
primary sex ratio, to have no effect, or to favor either under different circumstances.
We find that when a mating pair's behavior modifies mortality rates in favor of one
sex, but does not change the number of offspring produced in the mating, the primary
sex ratio will evolve a bias against the favored sex. However, when the total number
of offspring of a mating pair is significantly reduced as a consequence of their
prejudice, the primary sex ratio will evolve to favor the preferred sex. These results
hold irrespective of whether the sex ratio is distorted by the mother's, the father's
or the individual's own autosomal genes. The use of dynamic models of gene- culture
coevolution allows us to explore the evolution of alleles which distort the sex ratio,
as well as the final equilibrium states of the system. Gene-culture interactions
can provide equilibria different from those in purely genetic systems, slow the approach
to these equilibria by orders of magnitude, and move the primary (PSR) and the adult
sex ratio (ASR) away from any stable equilibrium for hundreds of generations.
Tuljapurkar, S., N. Li, and M. W. Feldman. 1995. High sex ratios in China's future.
Science 267:874-6.
Abstract: In China in recent years, male live births have exceeded those of females
by amounts far greater than those that occur naturally in human populations, a trend
with significant demographic consequences. The resulting imbalance in the first-marriage
market is estimated to be about 1 million males per year after 2010. These "excess"
males were not easily accommodated in models with substantial changes in first- marriage
patterns. The current sex ratio at birth has little effect on a couple's probability
of having at least one son, so future increases in the sex ratio may well occur,
especially given increasing access to sex-selective abortion.